MRI brain scanning sheds light on 'mind blindness'
Monday, 20 January, 2025
Aphantasia, or 鈥榤ind blindness鈥, is a condition where people cannot form mental images.
鈥淚f you try and imagine a beach, you can probably 鈥榮ee鈥 it in your mind鈥檚 eye,鈥 said Professor Joel Pearson from UNSW Sydney鈥檚 . 鈥淧eople with aphantasia don鈥檛 get that mental picture 鈥 it鈥檚 just blank.鈥
People with aphantasia also report a reduced ability to remember the past, imagine the future and even dream, according to a by Pearson鈥檚 Future Minds Lab at UNSW. While there is physiological evidence for aphantasia 鈥 discovered through previous studies using sensory measures 鈥 little is understood about the underlying neural mechanisms of the condition. Now, scientists from UNSW and South China Normal University have used MRI brain scanning to discover more about the neural processes in people with aphantasia.
In a breakthrough for understanding the condition, they found that when people with aphantasia try to conjure a mental image, the primary visual cortex 鈥 the part of the brain that processes picture-like visual information 鈥 is activated, but any images that are produced remain unconscious to the individual. This finding challenges the existing theory that activity in the primary visual cortex directly produces conscious visual imagery.
in psychology and neuroscience has demonstrated that visual experiences are closely linked to neural activity in the parts of the brain that process visual input at the earliest stages. The primary visual cortex is particularly important as it processes visual information in a picture-like format during perception and mental imagery, with its activation strength and size both tied to the .
Scientists have even based on neural activity in these areas, highlighting their role in creating subjective visual experiences. However, until now, it has been unclear whether people with aphantasia have this type of neural activity in their primary visual cortex.
The MRI study tested 14 participants with aphantasia and a control group of 18 participants who had typical mental imagery. The participants underwent two different tests to assess their visual imagery, followed by functional MRI (fMRI) scans. The scans utilised blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) fMRI, a technique that measures brain activity by detecting changes in blood oxygenation.
During the scans, participants viewed or imagined coloured striped patterns 鈥 which selectively activate the primary visual cortex. BOLD signals were recorded during either passive perception or attempts at imagining these patterns.
When passively looking at the striped patterns, participants with aphantasia showed a reduced BOLD response compared to the control group. When asked to mentally visualise the patterns, individuals with aphantasia reported little to no conscious imagery; however, distinct neural patterns were still generated during their imagery attempts. The researchers were even able to use algorithms to 鈥榙ecode鈥 what people with aphantasia were attempting to imagine from the brain activity in the primary visual cortex.
This suggests that there is indeed an image-specific representation in the brains of people with aphantasia; it just remains unconscious. These neural patterns are fundamentally different from perception and from those of people with mental imagery.
鈥淥ur results show that when someone with aphantasia tries to imagine, their brains still seem to create a representation in the early visual cortex. It鈥檚 like their brain is doing the maths but skipping the final step of showing the result on a screen,鈥 Pearson said.
鈥淭his tells us that mental imagery isn鈥檛 just about the brain 鈥榣ighting up鈥 鈥 it鈥檚 about how that activity is formatted into something we can actually experience.鈥
The results of the research,聽, open a door to understanding more about aphantasia, along with a range of other conditions involving altered mental imagery.
鈥淲e want and need to know more about how mental imagery is central to many psychological therapies and how superstrong imagery is associated with disorders such as schizophrenia and Parkinson鈥檚 disease,鈥 Pearson said. 鈥淯nderstanding the neurological basis of aphantasia could have a major impact on a range of disorders, and treatments to potentially give those with aphantasia mental imagery.鈥
While the study鈥檚 findings are significant, further research is still required, including studies with greater sample sizes.
鈥淣ext, we plan to figure out why exactly these representations in the visual brain don鈥檛 produce conscious imagery in those with aphantasia,鈥 Pearson said.
'Fake psychologist' who provided NDIS assessment convicted
A New South Wales woman who posed as a psychologist, providing an NDIS assessment, has been...
Residential eating disorder treatment centre is a Victorian first
Bridging a gap between community and hospital treatment, Victoria's first public residential...
Psychologist training pathway review launched
To reduce workforce shortages while maintaining standards, a review of the way psychologists are...